

Discover more from Moneyball Judaism
βKeep a light, hopeful heart. But Βexpect the worst.β β Joyce Carol Oates
Remember how most of us are average?
βSigh. How could I forget?β
And remember how most of us think we are above average despite the evidence?
βJosh, youβre a real buzzkill.β
Youβre welcome.
Believe it or not, Iβm an optimist. Or rather, Iβm an unusual optimist.Β
While I love doom-scrolling as much as the next survivalist (a la Roscoe Bartlett), ultimately, I define optimism as the belief that one can solve a problem if they are willing to understand it. While optimism tends to be associated with making hopeful predictions about the future, itβs time we explore why we tend to be more optimistic about the future than the data warrants and how optimism is as much about how we respond to negative events as it is about a sunny disposition.
Optimism Bias
How likely are you to die?
βJosh, I thought you said youβd make me feel better?β
Patience, young grasshopperβ¦
Actuarial tables aside, unless you are Enoch or Elijah,1, the odds of you dying eventually are 100%.
However, you probably never think about this. In fact, many of us secretly assume that maybe, just maybe, we will be the exception to the rule.
(I know I do.)
On Kol Nidre, the hazzan asks permission to pray with ΧΦΈΧ’Φ²ΧΦ·Χ¨Φ°ΧΦΈΧ Φ΄ΧΧ, what most siddurim translate as βthe transgressors.β I prefer a translation used by the late Rabbi Alan Lew that defines ΧΦΈΧ’Φ²ΧΦ·Χ¨Φ°ΧΦΈΧ Φ΄ΧΧ as βthose who are passing through.β2 All of us want to be the center of the universe, but the reality is that we are just mortals passing through this world, trying to do the best we can.
Like my unjustified belief that I could be immortal, the optimism bias is a misalignment between hopes and statistical reality. 100% of people die; I have no reason to believe I will be the exception, but I will likely believe I could be anyway. And when we turn to less likely yet still highly likely events, like losing a job or developing common health problems, Neil Feinstein finds that people are more likely to assume that bad events in the future will not happen to them, even when, statistically, most people will experience this event at some point.3
But I promised good news, so hereβs where things get interesting.
Our friend Tali Sharot argues that βThe ability to anticipate is the hallmark of cognition.β4 When I assess my or someone elseβs abilities, I focus on how well I can think through the future, anticipate problems before they become problems, and predict how they might respond when obstacles arise. And Sharot recognizes that βOne puzzle of optimism isβ¦that people maintain overly positive expectations despite a lifetime of experience with reality.β5
However, while Sharot recognizes that delusional optimism can be harmful, research also suggests that βoverestimating oneβs probability of success is advantageous in a world of uncertainty and competition.β6 While we are inclined to be optimistic when the odds suggest otherwise, those who understand how to harness optimism in a productive way have a systematic advantage that can only be utilized when rejecting magical thinking.Β While no amount of learning will make it more likely that you are above average from a statistical standpoint, recognizing this weakness is in and of itself a strength.
And this may be why our first step to harnessing optimism is recognizing how weβve misdefined it.
Learned Optimism
Martin Seligman has forgotten more about optimism than Iβll ever know.7 The first way he did that was by helping me understand how I had defined optimism and pessimism incorrectly.8
Seligman is credited as one of the founders of the "positive psychologyβ movement. In Learned Optimism, Seligman contends that the key distinction between optimists and pessimists is their "explanatory style." When confronted with a setback, a pessimist attributes it to "permanent causes" such as their own inadequacy or incompetence. Conversely, an optimist attributes setbacks to "temporary causes," prioritizing the situational context rather than assuming that setbacks result from unalterable traits.9 He writes:
βYour habitual way of explaining bad events, your explanatory style, is more than just the words you mouth when you fail.Β It is a habit of thought learned in childhood and adolescence.Β Your explanatory style stems directly from your view of your place in the world--whether you think you are valuable and deserving or worthless and hopeless.β10
Moreover, Seligman argues that this explanatory style extends to how optimists and pessimists explain positive events. The pessimist is more likely to explain positive events due to permanent causes (e.g., βIβm so brilliant!β), and the optimist is more likely to ascribe good events to specific actions (e.g., βI did X, Y, Z really well, and had A, B, and C happen, as a result.)
Turning to organizations, generally, it is easier to be a part of an organization constantly moving upward and expanding than one constantly struggling and contracting. However, when good things happen, and organizations ascribe success to brilliance rather than context, the organization quickly loses the ability to understand how to respond when things go poorly. And when an organization struggles, attributing those struggles to stupid, lazy, incompetent people only perpetuates a cycle that could be broken with a different perspective.
And this is why Iβm an unusual optimist. True or not, I believe that most problems can be solved if they are properly understood and addressed, and when I feel like I have a strong understanding of a problem, I will move mountains to make a difference. And I suspect Iβm not the only oneβ¦
Step Into the Happiness Lab
What I Read This Week
In Praise of Heroic Masculinity: I did not love this headline, although I enjoyed the article. Toxic masculinity is terrible for a reason, but strength can be transformative for all gender identities when properly harnessed.
Try Hard, But Not Too Hard: Another headline I donβt love. Apparently, 85% is the magic number for productivity. The article got me thinking, but you still cannot give 110%.11
Female Nonprofit CEOs Earn 73 Cents on the Dollar to Male CEOs: Sadly, Candidβs nonprofit compensation report finds that while the wage gap in smaller nonprofits decreased by six cents since 2011, the wage gap in larger nonprofits has actually increased. Buying the report is expensive, but the executive summary tells a disturbing story.
Narcissism is a Widely Misunderstood Phenomenon: βNarcissismβ is a widely used term, but far more complicated than its liberal usage would suggest. Hereβs a deeper analysis.
Text with Jesus: Nope, not a typo.
In the Tanakh, the text never tells us that Enoch or Elijah died, it only tells us that God βtookβ them. Could this be a rose by any other name, and βtakeβ is just a way of saying they died? Possibly. But the fact that we are not explicitly told of their deaths leads to incredible mystical speculation.
My favorite fact about Enoch? His son Methuselah βonlyβ lived 969 years, thereby making Methuselah the strangest disappointment in human history by not outliving his immortal father.
Ibid., R942-R943.
Ibid., R944.
Sharot devotes considerable attention to how optimism is helpful in terms of mental health, but itβs not where I want to focus in this article. That said, itβs fascinating and you should consider reading the entire article.
I have written about Seligman in several publications focusing on his understanding of explanatory style. I am doing my best not to self-plagiarize, but I cannot rule it out.
I would be remiss if I did not mention that Seligmanβs research has been used for some nefarious purposes, particularly regarding torture policies in the United States following September 11, 2011.
For a contentious exchange on this topic, read this piece in The New York Review Books by Tamsin Shaw, while being sure to read Seligmanβs response.
For a relatively balanced take on the darker side of Seligmanβs research, consider reading this piece in The New Yorker by our friend Maria Konnikova.
In essence, what Seligman found in his early research was that over time humans and animals can develop something called βlearned helplessness,β where they reach a psychological point where hope of progress seems so remote that they simply give up the idea of changing their circumstances. While Seligmanβs purpose in this research how to help patients overcome learned helplessness, post-9/11 reports regarding CIA torture policies found evidence that one of the tactics employed with suspected terrorists was inducing them to reach a state of learned helplessness such that they would cooperate with their interrogators.
Martin Seligman, Learned Optimism: How to Change Your Mind and Your Life (New York: Vintage, 2011, 45.
I would employ this distinction all the time when I was a classroom teacher when a student failed one of my tests. Failing a test is a bad thing. However, the pessimist looks at failing a test and explains this event by saying βIβm stupid.β In contrast, the optimist looks at failing a test and explains this event by saying βI did not study enough.β One explanation is permanent, the other contextual.
Ibid., 44.
Been a while since our last reference to The Simpsons.